Enter a name, company, place or keywords to search across this item. Then click "Search" (or hit Enter).
Genetics, Linguistics, and Prehistoric Migrations [DNA Analysis] (32 pages)

Copy the Page Text to the Clipboard

Show the Page Image

Show the Image Page Text


More Information About this Image

Get a Citation for Page or Image - Copy to the Clipboard

Go to the Previous Page (or Left Arrow key)

Go to the Next Page (or Right Arrow key)
Page: of 32

32 — Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology . Vol. 26, No. 1 (2006)
period, perhaps 5,000 years ago, noting the geographic
distribution of certain distinctive items of material culture
throughout much of the range known to have been
occupied historically by Uto-Aztecan speakers (Raab
and Howard 2002).
The coming of age of mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA)
genetic research by human biologists has provided an
important new means of examining past migrations
in many parts of North America (see Eshleman and
Smith, in press). Mitochondrial DNA is inherited only
maternally, so over many centuries mutations gradually
accumulate in daughter lineages, making it possible to
reconstruct ancestral types and derived lineages and
relate these to one another. All of the mtDNA lineages
in the Americas are associated with five recognized
“haplogroups” that originated earlier in prehistory
among human populations in Asia (Eshleman, Malhi,
and Smith 2003; Merriwether 2002; Schurr 2004; Torroni
2000). Observations of genetic affinities between ancient
peoples and ethnographically documented groups have
been matched with archaeological and linguistic evidence
to reconstruct population movements that led to patterns
observed at the time of European contact (Eshleman,
Malhi, and Smith 2003; Eshleman et al. 2004; Kaestle and
Smith 2001; Kemp et al. in press; Lorenz and Smith 1996,
1997; Malhi, Schulz, and Smith 2001; Malhi et al. 2002,
2003, 2004; O’Rourke, Hayes, and Carlyle 2000; Rubicz
et al. 2003; Schurr 2004).
Over the past thirteen years, the two coauthors
of this study have collaborated to study the mtDNA
from 126 separate California Indian matrilines (direct
female lineages). These lineages have been traced
through genealogical methods to specific native groups,
mostly in south-central and southern California, thus
providing a new database that elucidates the mosaic of
genetic relationships in the region. This paper will use
the sequences of the principal mtDNA hypervariable
segment (HIVS1) of 121 of these lineages in its analysis.
Earlier publications of portions of these data (Lorenz and
Smith 1996, 1997) and subsequent unpublished additions
have been used in a number of comparative studies
of native North American genetic relationships (e.g.,
Eshleman 2002; Eshleman et al. 2004; Kaestle and Smith
2001; Kowta 2003; Malhi et al. 2002, 2003; O’Rourke,
Hayes, and Carlyle 2000). Continuing genealogical
research has revised the ethnolinguistic group affiliations
of some of these previously reported samples, and the
accumulation of additional samples has augmented our
database significantly. The expanded and refined database
produced by our continuing efforts now provides us with
a clearer picture of mtDNA diversity among California
Indian groups than has heretofore been available.
LANGUAGE CHANGE IN
CALIFORNIA PREHISTORY
At the time of European contact, California was made
up of a veritable patchwork of local groups often called
tribelets (Kroeber 1963). In some regions, especially in
areas of higher population density supported by fishing
economies, there existed different levels of sociocultural
integration and hierarchical relations among these
local groups (Bean 1978; Johnson 1988, 2000; Jorgensen
1980). Contributing to the cultural mosaic represented
by different tribelets and regional differences in sociopolitical organization was a high degree of linguistic
differentiation. Some 88 distinct languages were spoken
between the southern tip of Baja California and Oregon
(Goddard 1996a, 1996b; Laylander 1997; Mithun 1999).
These have been classified into fourteen language families
and seven isolate languages. A number of these families
and individual languages traditionally have been grouped
into two macro-units or superfamilies, Hokan and
Penutian, although the reality of these larger taxonomic
entities remains hypothetical and is a subject of current
investigation and debate among specialists in comparative
linguistics. One interpretation is that these macro-units
appear to represent ancient language families that
became dispersed and evolved into descendant families
and isolated languages through linguistic processes (Golla
2000a). Linguists and archaeologists have been interested
in explaining the distribution of languages in the Pacific
Coast region and adjacent Great Basin and have devoted
much effort to reconstructing linguistic prehistory and
proposing likely cultural processes (e.g., Bettinger and
Baumhoff 1982; Breschini 1983; Bright and Bright 1976;
Golla 2000b; Hill 2002; Hughes 1992; Jackson 1989; King
1986; Krantz 1977; Levy 1997; Madsen and Rhode 1994;
Moratto 1984; Nichols 1988; Whistler 1977, 1978, 1988).
It is often assumed that populations speaking related
languages might share genetic lineages in common,
because they once had belonged to the same group